Types of Biases
We have
listed what we think are some of the most frequent and important biases that occur and have included examples where we thought they
would help explain the bias. Are there more types of bias
than what we've listed here? Yes, there are a whole lot,
and we have a link at the bottom of this page that will take you
to a list.
Implicit Bias:
Definition: There are various definitions of implicit
bias, all of which include the idea that biases can occur outside of
conscious awareness and/or conscious control.
As noted in the introduction to biases, and the
discussion of the neurological bases for biases (see links below),
humans recognize patterns in order to navigate the complexities of the
world we live in. This, along with socio-cultural upbringing, is
seen as cause and basis of implicit biases.
Given the extent to which people are often unable to see
the degree to which they might actually have biases (which some might
call "preferences"), research into this topic has been an important
exploration of this phenomenon.
To
take an online test for Implicit Biases
Confirmation Bias (AKA "Believing is Seeing") -
Everything you look for and
all that you perceive has a way of proving whatever you already
believe.
This is a really, really,
really, really important kind of bias. We have discussed
it somewhat in our article about the scientific method, but we
want to devote more attention to it here in a separate article.
Definition: Confirmation Bias is the
tendency to search for or interpret information in a way that
confirms one's preconceptions. Often, this involves
ignoring any information that might invalidate one's
perceptions, and it leads to the logical fallacy called "Cherry
Picking."
More specifically, confirmation bias says that
when you seek information (facts, data) about reality, you only
look for, or recognize that which supports what you already
believe, and if you find it, you stop there. You either disattend, or ignore
, or don't look further and deeper to avoid finding anything that
might contradict your preexisting beliefs and perceptions about
reality.
So, for example, If you think it is really safe
to drive over the speed limit, you'll not take into account data
regarding auto accidents caused by excessive speed.
Or, if you believe private
gun ownership reduces crime, you only look at data comparing two
similar towns, one that allows concealed carry and one that does
not, and find the former has a lower crime rate, thereby
confirming what you already believe, and you don't look to
compare this data with areas where gun ownership and crime
are high (e.g., some inner city areas with high gang presence).
Or when you hear that 60% of
the population opposes some legislation, without looking further
to find out that only half of that group opposes it because the
don't like what it does, while the other half opposes it because
it doesn't do as much as they want it to do, which means only
the first half (i.e., only 30%) want to overturn the
legislation.
In some cases, this bias can be
rather harmless, but in others, it can lead to disastrous
results, gross misperceptions that lead to bad decisions, that
can even result in death.
Thing is, when folks look for
confirmation of preexisting notions and assumptions, they can
not only ignore that which might disprove their preconceptions,
but they may embrace things that are outright false (like "urban
legends").
To make this concrete, there
can be large number of facts and data about something, that
folks can manipulate statistically to present an inaccurate
picture, but it can easily be embraced by those seeking
confirmation of something they already consider to be true.
Status
Quo Bias - AKA
Aversion to Change Bias, this bias is a preference for keeping
things the way they are, it includes a wish to avoid risk that
might occur should things change. In other words, this is
a bias against change.
Here's an example
involving automotive insurance in New Jersey and Pennsylvania.
This is something that really happened in the early 1990s.
Both states initiated tort
reform programs with two options for their automobile insurance.
One was an expensive options that included the full right to
sue, and the other was a less expensive option which had limited
rights to sue. We don't know why they did it this way, but
the two states offered one option as a default option.
BUT they did not offer the
SAME as default. In New Jersey, the cheaper option with
limited right to sue was given as default, while in Pennsylvania
the more expensive option with full rights to sue was given as
default.
To make this clear, default =
status quo. If people chose "default," they were in effect
choosing "status quo."
Now what makes this
interesting was that the two states did NOT offer the same
option as default (status quo), AND in both states folks chose
the default option over the other one. In New Jersey the
majority chose the cheaper option, while in Pennsylvania they
chose the more expensive option.
Bias blind spot
–
the tendency to see oneself as less biased than other people.
Choice-supportive bias – the tendency to remember one's choices as being better than
they actually were.
Self-serving bias
-
the tendency to claim more responsibility for successes than
failures. It may also manifest itself as a tendency for
people to evaluate ambiguous information in a way beneficial to
their interests.
Attentional Bias –
this has to do with what we choose to pay attention to. It
is an implicit cognitive bias defined as the tendency of emotionally
dominant stimuli in one's environment to preferentially draw and
hold attention. Some might call the the "Cup half empty vs
the cup half full" bias. It sounds a little like
confirmation bias, since it relates to what we choose to pay
attention to, but it doesn't really concern what we ignore.
Some call it the "Oh, look, something shiny" bias.
Deciding to vote for a candidate who has a broad array of goals,
some of which may actually not be in the particular voters best interest, because
one of the goals is important to the voter. This could
include an elderly voter choosing to vote for someone who wants
to eliminate social security, because that candidate is opposed
to equal rights for homosexuals.
Bandwagon effect –
the tendency to do (or believe) things because many other people
do (or believe) the same. Related to groupthink and herd
behavior. Some like to call this the "Lemming effect."
Believing something
many folks believe and saying, "If they all believe it, it must
be true."
Hostile media effect - the tendency to see a media report as being biased due to
one's own strong partisan views.
When a news story
reports something negative about a candidate from a person's
own political party, saying that report is obviously biased, because
the news outlet favors the other political party.
Hindsight bias -
sometimes called the "I-knew-it-all-along" effect, or "Monday-morning-quarterbacking"
- the tendency to see past events as being predictable.
That is, after something happens, looking back and saying, "Oh,
they should have known better than to do that, because what
happened next was predictable," when, in fact, it might not have
been predictable at all beforehand.
After a peace
negotiation fails because one side refused to participate,
saying the should have known in advance that would happen.
Belief bias - when
one's evaluation of the logical strength of an argument is
biased by one's belief in the truth or falsity of the
conclusion.
Fred's hard work
is the reason he got rich. If one believes that riches
only come to those who work hard, this appears to be a logical
statement. However, it just happens that Fred inherited
all his money. It was strictly luck - he didn't work for a
penny of it.
Attribution bias - Here's
rather self-centered form of bias. It involves
attributing different causes for one's own behavior than for the
behavior of others. Simply put, in this bias, we say that
our own behavior is mostly caused by context, the environmental
factors occurring that motivate our actions.
On the other hand, we don't see the behavior of
others as that context driven. Instead we attribute the
causes to their personality, or some other mental attribute,
such as intelligence.
Fred is walking down the street. Behind Fred, a
car stopping for a light that has suddenly turned red, skids
toward the intersection. Fred jumps off the sidewalk onto
the lawn of the house he is walking next to. Now, someone
seeing Fred do this might conclude that he is fearful (a
personality attribute), but Fred might well simply see it as a
reaction to a noise suggesting a car might come careening up
onto the sidewalk.
Another example: We see Fred trip
on something on the sidewalk, we say this resulted from the fact
that Fred is clumsy, or too inattentive. But if we trip on
something in the sidewalk, we say it's because there's a crack
in the sidewalk.
This is NOT
to say that one, or the other is appropriate. We humans do
exhibit some repeating patterns of behavior in different context
which can be seen as "personality," but at the same time, it is
the context that activates these behaviors.
What we ARE
saying is that this tendency to be biased in our attributions
can limit our ability to perceive realities about ourselves and
other people.
Okay, okay,
you say, but why is this "attribution bias" so important that it
has to be discussed here? The answer is: Because when it
comes to viewing others it can lead to harmful stereotyping,
especially when the personality attribution is a disservice to
the other (e.g., he's just "lazy," and so are all the people in
his ethnic group).
Blame the victim
bias - closely related to the fundamental attribution bias,
this is the tendency to say that people only get what they
deserve. This is a part of a general bias toward personal
responsibility, that is, ignoring the role of the environment,
including chance and opportunity, and holding everyone personally responsible
for everything that happens to them.
This is a favorite tactic
in politics, where politicians do everything possible to thwart
their opponents, then blame the opponents for not succeeding.
Fred got robbed,
because he was walking down a dark street alone. Or,
we blocked every bill in Congress presented by the other side, but if they
had done a better job, their bills would have passed--in other
words, it's their fault the bills didn't pass.
Many of these
biases form the psychological bases for committing a
number of logical fallacies.
To
read about the neuropsychological basis for biases
To
read about bias detection
To
see a long list of cognitive biases (will open in a new window,
or tab)
To
view our Logical Fallacies Page
To
view our Science Page